Introduction

Exercise and exposure of the human body to hypoxia are well-known physiological and environmental stressors that can modify the functions of the central nervous system (CNS), but typically their effect is the opposite. Hypoxia is thought to have detrimental effects on the CNS by decreasing arterial pressure of O2 (PaO2) and arterial saturation of O2 (SaO2) (Kolb et al., 2004) leading to neurological deficits and impairment of cognitive performance at rest (Wilson et al., 2009). Furthermore, the turnover of several neurotransmitters can be altered under hypoxia compared to normoxia, adversely affecting cognitive functions (Gibson et al., 1981; Kramer et al., 2006; Levitt and Gutin, 1971). Less clear-cut results relate to physical effort under hypoxia, as under such conditions both beneficial and detrimental effects on cognitive performance have been reported. This applies primarily to research published in recent years: some studies have reported cognitive improvement in response to combined effects of acute exercise and hypoxia (Komiyama et al., 2017; Seo et al., 2017), whereas other studies have found that cognitive performance was impaired under such conditions (Lefferts et al., 2016; Ochi et al., 2018). Nevertheless, there does seem to be a research-based consensus, supported by a growing body of evidence, suggesting that acute exercise at moderate intensity under hypoxia does indeed improve cognitive performance (Chang et al., 2012). The aforementioned phenomenon may be, at least partially, explained by increased cardiac output and cerebral blood flow, which are pivotal in maintaining oxygen delivery to the CNS (Aimslie and Subudhi, 2014). The above described inconsistent findings were above all obtained in studies conducted in young sedentary men, and the resulting differences are attributed to methodological differences and differing experimental designs (Dinoff et al., 2016).

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which is believed to affect cognitive functions, among other things, is the most abundant neurotrophin located in the hippocampus, cortex, midbrain, thalamus (amygdala), hypothalamus, striatum, pons and medulla (Binder and Scharfman, 2004; Cunha et al., 2010). Importantly, dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra and striatum, e.g., structures involved in movement regulation, have been found to be the main source of BDNF secretion (Ventriglia et al., 2013). Regular long-term repeated physical exercise and/or moderate to high intensity training induces an increased level of BDNF and TrkB receptors in the brain regions responsible for motor activity (Zoladz and Pilc, 2010). Acute aerobic exercise increases the concentration of the BDNF in the blood (Szuhany et al., 2015) in both trained (Piotrowicz et al., 2020) and sedentary individuals (Piotrowicz et al., 2019). This increase depends on the intensity and duration of the single bout of exercise or the training process (Liu and Nusslock, 2018). In other studies, an increase in serum BDNF was reported after high-intensity interval training (HIIT) (Helm et al., 2017). Notably, high-intensity exercise performed at short intervals has been found to be more effective than continuous high-intensity exercise for increasing serum BDNF (Jiménez-Maldonado et al., 2018), while a 12-week resistance training exercise program of healthy individuals was not found to cause an increase in basal BDNF concentrations (Williams and Ferris, 2012). Keeping in mind that the vast majority of studies have provided evidence of the beneficial effects of physical exercise on cognitive functions, with a simultaneous increase in BDNF levels in the blood, this suggests that BDNF may be involved in improving cognitive function (Ferris et al., 2007; Gold et al., 2003; Hung at al., 2018).

One of the first studies on BDNF release during exercise under hypoxia showed no additive effect on the serum BDNF concentration (Van Cutsem et al., 2015). A previous study (Piotrowicz et al., 2020) reported that maximal exercise performed under both normoxia and hypoxia conditions, corresponding to 3000 m altitude (FiO2 = 14.7%), caused a similar increase in peripheral BDNF concentration in endurance athletes. However, an increase in the BDNF did not prevent cognitive impairment under hypoxia, as measured by choice reaction time (CRT) and the number of correct reactions (NCR) (Piotrowicz et al., 2020). These limited data indicate that the relationship between BDNF secretion and cognitive function under hypoxia is unclear and poorly understood.

The effects of habitual exercise or training processes on the human body are thought to be the result of repeated exercise, and therefore may be associated with cumulative acute responses to exercise. Importantly, the chronic effects of exercise can be modified using the same acute exercise, by changing the exercise mode, strength, duration, and/or frequency. One can expect that this may also involve different adaptive changes in the brain in response to different training processes. Therefore, adaptive changes in the brain triggered by strength training may be different from those caused by endurance training (Hashimoto et al., 2021). For example, studies demonstrated that some metabolic factors (e.g., lactate, myokines), released during muscle contraction depending on the exercise mode, activated unmyelinated group IV afferents, whereas mechanical-sensitive, myelinated group III afferents were preferentially stimulated during the production of muscle strength (Gandevia, 2001). Studies in animals have revealed that adaptive changes in the brain triggered by endurance training affect the serotonergic system (Langfort et al., 2006). Other research on the effects of aerobic and strength training on BDNF levels and neuroplasticity has found that both endurance and resistance-training result in similar stimulating effects on BDNF levels in rats (Vilela et al., 2017).

It is not known whether acute exercise to exhaustion under hypoxic conditions will elicit a different response in individuals who exhibit adaptive changes in the brain caused by endurance or resistance training. This phenomenon is not yet fully understood considering psychomotor skills and possible involvement of BDNF in this phenomenon due to a small number of studies. In this regard, the aim of the present study was to examine whether a single bout of maximal exercise to volitional exhaustion, performed under moderate normobaric hypoxia, would affect psychomotor performance in strength-trained athletes, compared to endurance-trained athletes and individuals leading a sedentary lifestyle. For this purpose, we measured the peripheral level of BDNF, CRT and NCR as indices of psychomotor performance during exercise to volitional exhaustion. In addition, we examined the level of selected circulating biochemical factors, such as cortisol (COR), lactate (LA), nitric oxide (NO), endothelin-1 (ET-1), and catecholamines, because they are known to affect BDNF expression / production, and their expression can be influenced by both exercise and hypoxia.

Methods

Participants

Ten strength-trained (S) athletes (age 22.0 ± 1.7 years; body height 180.7 ± 3.9 cm; body mass 79.6 ± 6.4 kg; fat content 11.3 ± 4.0%) and ten endurance-trained (E) athletes (age 21 ± 1.3 years; body height 180.2 ± 6.7 cm; body mass 69.6 ± 6.8 kg; fat content 9.1 ± 3.1% ), as the experimental groups, plus ten healthy men leading a sedentary lifestyle (age 20.1 ± 1.2 years; body height 177.2 ± 8.9 cm; body mass 71.4 ± 10.6 kg; fat content 12.1 ± 4.5%), as a control (C) group, were recruited for the study. All participants had current valid medical examinations and showed no contraindications to participating in the experiment.

Each participant declared that for at least one month prior to testing, they had not taken either medications or dietary supplements, and each provided written informed consent prior to the study commencement. The experimental procedures and related risks were explained to all the participants verbally, and they were able to withdraw from the study at any time. The investigation was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Ethics Committee for Scientific Research at the Jerzy Kukuczka Academy of Physical Education in Katowice, Katowice, Poland (no. 5/2013, approval date: 26 June 2013).

Participants were tested on two occasions, five days apart. On each occasion, participants underwent two graded exercise tests until volitional exhaustion (EVE) on an ergocycle, performed under normobaric normoxic and normobaric hypoxic conditions (3000 m asl). Participants were allocated to the normoxic or hypoxic conditions randomly, by means of a computer-generated list (Urbaniak and Plous, 2013). Before each test, the body mass and body composition of each participant was determined using electrical impedance measurements, by means of an Inbody 720 body composition analyzer (Biospace Co., Tokyo, Japan).

To create hypoxic conditions, representing the equivalent of 3000 m asl altitude (FiO2 = 14.7%), we used a normobaric hypoxia chamber (Losa Hyp/Hyop-2/3NU system, Lowoxygen Systems, Berlin, Germany) maintained at the Hypoxia Laboratory.

Ergocycle Graded Exercise Test to Volitional Exhaustion

The graded exercise tests were carried out on an Excalibur Sport ergocycle (Lode BV, Groningen Netherlands), beginning at a workload of 40 W, which was increased by 40 W every 3 min until volitional exhaustion. When a particular participant discontinued the test before completing a given workload, then the maximum workload for that test was calculated by the formula WRmax = WRk + (t/T × WRp) (Kuipers et al., 1985), where WRk represented the previous workload, t represented exercise duration with the workload until premature failure, T represented duration of each workload, and WRp was the workload increment by which exercise intensity was increased during the test.

During the tests, the heart rate (HR), minute ventilation (VE), breathing frequency (BF), oxygen uptake (VO2) and carbon dioxide content in expired air (VCO2) were recorded for each participant by means of a MetaMax 3B gas analyzer (Cortex, Leipzig, Germany). Fingertip capillary blood samples for the assessment of LA concentration (Biosen C-line Clinic, EKF- diagnostic GmbH, Barleben, Germany) were drawn at rest and at the end of each testing stage.

Psychomotor Performance Determination

Choice reaction time and NCR were selected as indices of psychomotor performance. The CRT console was mounted on the wall in front of the ergometer, at the eye level, 1.5 m away from the participant. The test included 15 positive (red light or a sound) and 15 negative (green or yellow light) stimuli displayed in randomized order, in 1s to 4 s intervals. Participants were asked to press and then to release, as quickly as possible, the button of the switch device kept in their right hand in response to the red light stimulus, the button in the left hand in response to the sound stimulus, and not to react in either way to the negative stimuli. The total time for each CRT was 107 s. The stimuli and participants’ responses were recorded using the reaction time measuring device (MRK 432, ZEAM, Zabrze, Poland), and determined to the nearest 0.01 s. The results are presented as the mean reaction time for all 15 responses to positive stimuli. Participants had been familiarized with the testing procedure a week before the study, by practicing the task both at rest and during cycling.

Venous Blood

On the day of the EVE, each participant was cannulated into their antecubital vein, 15 min prior to having breakfast. Two samples of venous blood were then collected at each time point: 10 min later, immediately after cessation of each ergocycle test, and again 1 h after each ergocycle test. Of each pair of samples, one was drawn using an ethylenediaminetetraacetic (EDTA) tube (for morphology analysis); the other was drawn using an anti-coagulant tube and processed for serum for the other biochemical assays (BDNF, ET-1, C, catecholamines). After 30 min, blood samples were centrifuged at 1500 × g for 15 min. The serum samples so obtained were kept stored at −80°C until they were analyzed.

Determination of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor, Cortisol and Endothelin-1 Concentrations

Assays of blood serum levels of BDNF, C, and ET-1 were performed using a commercially available Quantikine ELISA kit (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) in compliance with the procedure supplied by the manufacturer. This method allows for measurements of BDNF, C, and ET-1 in the range of 0.372–4000 pg/mL, 0.030–100 ng/mL, and 0.031–50 ng/mL, respectively. The intra-assay coefficient of variance was <4.0%, <8%, <4%, respectively. To quantify the level, a standard curve was performed using a standard solution.

Determination of Catecholamines by HPLC Method

Assays of blood serum levels of adrenalin (A), noradrenaline (NA) and dopamine (DA) were carried out using a high-performance liquid chromatograph (HPLC, Gynkotek, Copenhagen, Denmark) with electrochemical detection using a Coulochem III model 520 (ESSA, Copenhagen, Denmark). Serum samples were mixed with 0.1 M perchloric acid containing 22.5 ng/mL ascorbic acid (ASC, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). After being centrifuged at 15,000 g for 10 min at 4°C, the supernatant was filtered through a nylon syringe filter (Millipore, 0.22 μm, Merck KGaA, Darmstadt, Germany). Samples of 20 μL filtrate were then injected into a high-performance liquid chromatography system (Gynkotek, Copenhagen, Denmark) equipped with a Hypersil Gold (15 cm × 4.6 mm) column (Thermo Electron Corporation, Kleinostheim, Germany). The samples were eluted by a mobile phase comprising 107 mM of Na2HPO4 × 2H2O, 107 mM citric acid, 0.3 mM octane-1 sulfonic acid sodium salt (OSA), 0.2 μM of EDTA, pH, 4.6, 1.5% methanol and 1.5% acetonitrile, at a flow rate of 0.8 mL/min. The column temperature was set at 25°C. Peaks were identified by electrochemical detection (Coulochem III, ESSA, Copenhagen, Denmark) with potentials set at E1 = −50 mV and E2 = +400 mV. Data were collected and analyzed using Chromeleon software running on a PC (Gynkotek, Copenhagen, Denmark). DA and 5-HT levels in the sample were calculated by extrapolating the peak area from a standard curve.

Determination of Nitric Oxide in Serum

Nitrate/nitrite (NO) concentration was determined in serum using a Total Nitric Oxide and Nitrate/Nitrite assay (kat. No.KGE001; R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) according to the manufacturer’s instruction. This assay determines nitric oxide concentrations based on the enzymatic conversion of nitrate to nitrite by nitrate reductase. The reaction is followed by colorimetric detection of nitrite as an azo dye product of the Griess Reaction. The Griess Reaction is based on the twostep diazotization reaction in which acidified NO2- produces a nitrosating agent, which reacts with sulfanilic acid to produce the diazonium ion. This ion is then coupled to N-(1-naphthyl) ethylenediamine to form the chromophoric azo-derivative which absorbs light at 540–570 nm. The optical density (OD) in each 96-well plate was measured using a microplate reader (Ex 800 TK Biotech, Taipei City, Taiwan) at 540 nm. The concentration of nitrite was calculated by the nitrite standard curve.

Statistical Analysis

The results of the study were analyzed using Statistica 13.3 software (TIBCO Software Inc., USA). The results are presented as arithmetic means (x) and standard deviations (SD). The statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Prior to all statistical analyses, the normality of the distribution of variables was verified using the Shapiro-Wilk test. The intergroup differences between the research trials, comparisons of repeated measurements and differences between the groups were assessed by the analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures. When significant differences were found, the Tukey’s post-hoc test was used.

Results

Incremental Test Results and Blood Lactate Concentration

ANOVA with repeated measures showed a significant interaction (conditions × groups) for the maximal workload (WRmax, F = 3.5; p < 0.05) and VO2max (F = 91.2; p < 0.001) during the incremental test. Statistical analysis did not show a significant interaction (conditions × groups) for delta values of blood lactate concentration (∆LA) during the incremental test; however, ∆ values of LA in hypoxia were significantly different compared to normoxia (F = 40.09, p < 0.001).

The Tukey’s post-hoc test revealed that WRmax (p < 0.001) decreased significantly due to hypoxia exposition (3000 m) in all groups (by 14.1% in E athletes, 11.7% in S athletes, and 16.7% in the C group) (Figure 1a). Moreover, WRmax values in the E group were significantly (p < 0.01) higher compared to the S and C groups under normoxia and hypoxia (Figure 1a). The same trend of changes was also observed in VO2max values under hypoxia. The statistical analysis showed a significant (p < 0.001) decrease in VO2max values under hypoxia (3000 m) in all tested groups (by 16.7% in E athletes, 14.3% in S athletes, and 18.2% in the C group; Figure 1b). The values of VO2max in the E group were significantly (p < 0.001) higher compared to the values in S and C groups under normoxia and hypoxia (Figure 1b). However, ∆LA during the incremental test under hypoxia significantly (p < 0.05) increased in S and E groups, by 7.5% and 9.7%, respectively, compared to normoxia (Figure 2). There were no statistically significant changes in ∆LA values in the C group under hypoxia.

Figure 1

Mean values of the maximal workload (WRmax) (a) and VO2max (b) during the incremental test performed under different conditions. S – strength-trained; E – endurance-trained; C – control; *** p < 0.001 (compared to normoxia); ** p < 0.001 (compared to normoxia); # p < 0.01 (compared to C and S groups under normoxia), ## p < 0.01 (compared to S and C groups under hypoxia)

https://jhk.termedia.pl/f/fulltexts/168282/JHK-87-168282-g001_min.jpg
Figure 2

Mean values of ∆LA after the incremental test performed under different conditions.

* p < 0.05 (compared to normoxia); *** p < 0.001 (compared to normoxia)

https://jhk.termedia.pl/f/fulltexts/168282/JHK-87-168282-g002_min.jpg

Choice Reaction Time and the Number of Correct Reactions

ANOVA with repeated measures for time × group interactions showed statistically significant differences in CRT under normoxia (F = 15.46, p < 0.001) and hypoxia. Similarly, there was a significant difference in NCR under normoxia (F = 3.98; p < 0.0) and hypoxia (3000 m) (F = 18.18; p < 0.001).

The Tukey’s post-hoc test revealed a significant (p < 0.05) increase in CRT immediately after the test, and a significant (p < 0.05) decrease after 3 min of recovery only in the E group under normoxia and hypoxia (Figures 3a, 3b). There were no significant differences in CRT in the S and C groups under either condition, as well as between conditions (normoxia vs. hypoxia) in all groups.

Figure 3

Choice reaction time (CRT) at rest, during maximal effort (max) and after 3 min of recovery under (a) normoxia, and (b) hypoxia (3000 m). *** p < 0.001

https://jhk.termedia.pl/f/fulltexts/168282/JHK-87-168282-g003_min.jpg

There were no significant differences in NCR in the S and C groups under either condition, or between conditions (normoxia vs. hypoxia) in all groups (Figures 4a and 4b). The post-hoc test showed a significant (p < 0.05) decrease in NCR immediately after the test, and a significant (p < 0.05) increase after 3 min of recovery only in the E group under hypoxia (Figure 4b).

Figure 4

Number of correct reactions (NCR) at rest, during maximal effort (max) and after 3 min of recovery in (a) normoxia, and (b) hypoxia (3000 m). * p < 0.05

https://jhk.termedia.pl/f/fulltexts/168282/JHK-87-168282-g004_min.jpg

Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor and Selected Biochemical Variables

ANOVA with repeated measures for time × group interactions showed statistically significant differences in BDNF concentration under normoxia (F = 2.55, p < 0.05) and hypoxia (F = 34.93, p < 0.001), ET-1 concentration under normoxia (F = 4.27; p < 0.01) and hypoxia (F = 3.53; p < 0.05), NO2- under normoxia (F = 5.01; p < 0.01) and hypoxia (F = 10.72; p < 0.0001), COR only under normoxia (F = 2.89; p < 0.06), and A under normoxia (F = 8.18; p < 0.001) and hypoxia (F = 18.75; p< 0.001). There were no significant time × group interactions for DA and NA concentration under normoxia and hypoxia. However, NA concentration under hypoxia was significantly different (F = 12.62, p < 0.001) compared to normoxia.

ANOVA with repeated measures did not show a significant condition × group interaction for BDNF concentration at rest, immediately after the test and after 1 h of recovery; however, BDNF concentration immediately after the test under hypoxia was significantly different (F = 11.48, p < 0.01) compared to normoxia.

The Tukey’s post-hoc test showed that BDNF concentration under normoxia significantly increased immediately after the test in the E group (by 30.5%; p < 0.05) and the C group (by 34.1%; p < 0.01). Additionally, BDNF concentration decreased significantly (p < 0.01) after 1 h of recovery, by 24.4% in the C group, and 39.5% in the S group under normoxia (Figure 5a). Under hypoxia conditions (3000 m), BDNF concentration increased significantly (p < 0.05) immediately after the test in all groups (by 46.8% in E athletes, 32.1% in S athletes, and 42.3% in the C group). After 1 h of recovery, BDNF concentration significantly decreased (p < 0.001) by 40.8% in the E group, and by 29.7% in the C group. There was no significant decrease in BDNF concentration after 1 h of recovery in the S group in the hypoxia environment (Figure 5b). The Tukey’s post-hoc test revealed that BDNF concentration immediately after the test in the E group was significantly higher (by 6.5%, p < 0.05) under hypoxia compared to normoxia.

Figure 5

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) serum concentration at rest, immediately after the test (max) and after 1 h of recovery under (a) normoxia, and (b) hypoxia (3000 m).

*p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.

https://jhk.termedia.pl/f/fulltexts/168282/JHK-87-168282-g005_min.jpg

Additionally, the post-hoc test showed that ET-1 significantly increased immediately after the test (by 44.9%, p < 0.001), and significantly decreased after 1 h of recovery (by 47.8%, p < 0.001), compared to the resting level in the C group under normoxia (Table 1). Under hypoxia, ET-1 increased significantly immediately after the test only in the C group (by 35.2%, p < 0.001). There were no significant differences in other groups in ET-1 concentration (Table 2).

Table 1

Mean values of selected biochemical variables registered under normoxia condition at rest, immediately after the incremental test (max) and after 1 h of recovery (after 1 h).

VariablesNormoxia
at restmaxafter 1 h
groupx ± SDMex ± SDMex ± SDMe
ET-1
[pg/ml]
S6.8 ± 4.54.88.4 ± 4.16.56.7 ± 4.05.4
E2.5 ± 0.92.52.4 ± 1.82.32.0 ± 1.11.8
C6.9 ± 3.16.710 ± 2.8***11.310.2 ± 4.7**8.6
NO2-
[pg/ml]
S41.5 ± 28.528.739.7 ± 24.328.147.1 ± 13.546.8
E29.8 ± 12.227.547.7 ± 22.836.931.5 ± 12.625.7
C49.1 ± 21.554.959.5 ± 20.648.231.7 ± 18.2#27.7
C
[pg/ml]
S7.1 ± 2.76.311 ± 3.4***11.17.9 ± 3.1#7.4
E7.8 ± 3.06.711.1 ± 3.511.39.4 ± 2.99.7
C7.8 ± 3.06.611.0 ± 3.510.39.5 ± 2.99.7
NA
[pg/l]
S127.9 ± 72.0136.3263.1 ± 126.8240.7262.5 ± 93.4237.1
E363.6 ± 207.8366.4566.8 ± 261.3**575.6422.1 ± 212.9471.8
C364.5 ± 133.3350.3439.9 ± 123.8422.1442.3 ± 89.7402.6

A
[pg/l]
S18.6 ± 12.614.1189.5 ± 92.7**165.6174.3 ± 50.1***163.1
E53.9 ± 31.046.0383.5 ± 197.2***351.8122.5 ± 102.0##117.0
C31.9 ± 8.029.2188.5 ± 32.0***189.5133.4 ± 22.1139.7
DA
[pg/l]
S12.0 ± 7.211.817.5 ± 10.113.516.3 ± 10.813.1
E4.3 ± 1.74.28.7 ± 6.17.65.4 ± 1.35.2
C13.8 ± 2.514.319.9 ± 4.218.814.8 ± 1.415.1

[i] S – strength-trained; E – endurance-trained; C – control; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001 compared to rest; # p < 0.01; ## p < 0.001 compared to max; & p < 0.08 compared to rest

Table 2

Mean values of selected biochemical variables registered under hypoxia condition at rest, immediately after the incremental test (max) and after 1 h of recovery (after 1 h).

VariablesHypoxia 3000 m
groupat restmaxafter 1 h
x ± SDMex ± SDMex ± SDMe
ET-1
[pg/ml]
S6.8 ± 3.36.48.1 ± 2.77.58.2 ± 3.47.4
E2.3 ± 0.82.52.3
± 1.0
2.52.6 ± 1.22.3
C9.1 ± 4.07.612.3 ± 5.8***9.710.6 ± 3.911.9
NO2-
[pg/ml]
S46.7 ± 21.736.238.9 ± 6.642.651.8 ± 23.141.8
E35.0 ± 16.740.153.3 ± 16.6*51.030.2 ± 9.5#28.0
C29.2 ± 12.331.057.3 ± 23.8***56.622.4 ± 5.5#23.0
C
[pg/ml]
S6.1 ± 2.46.19.1 ± 2.68.98.1 ± 9.25.9
E6.6 ± 2.36.48.3 ± 2.58.88.1 ± 4.37.1
C6.6 ± 2.26.49.9 ± 2.09.29.4 ± 2.99.7
NA
[pg/l]
S367.0 ± 198.9470.9732.3 ± 326.9746.6639.9 ± 231.5588.4
E326.3 ± 158.9238.61047.6 ± 1080.6*348.4591.5 ± 374.1471.8
C149.9 ± 43.2136.0392.3 ± 167.7336.6416.9 ± 78.4399.6

A
[pg/l]
S31.5 ± 13.525.5327.6 ± 91.5&318.9301.1 ± 130.0333.6
E204.4 ± 74.5228.51498.1 ± 627.0**1539.1856.9 ± 261.4*#763.7
C36.2 ± 7.437.9271.3 ± 67.0302.5191.4 ± 40.7198.9
DA
[pg/l]
S42.8 ± 20.038.350.7 ± 25.839.156.8 ± 21.555.8
E7.3 ± 2.66.812.5 ± 2.914.312.6 ± 5.513.7
C13.5 ± 3.715.120.1 ± 2.521.516.7 ± 1.316.9

[i] S – strength-trained; E – endurance-trained; C – control; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001 compared to rest; # p < 0.01; ## p < 0.001 compared to max; & p < 0.08 compared to rest

Under normoxia, NO2- decreased significantly (p < 0.01) by 46.7%, after 1 h of recovery compared to values after exercise in the C group (Table 1). Hoverer, under hypoxia, NO2- increased significantly (p < 0.05) immediately after the test in the E and C groups, by 52.3% and 96.2%, respectively. After 1 h of recovery under hypoxia condition, NO2- concentration decreased significantly (by 43.3%, p < 0.01) only in the E group. Serum concentration of COR increased significantly (by 56.3%, p < 0.001) immediately after the test compared to initial concentration in the C group, and decreased significantly (by 28.8%, p < 0.001) in this group after 1 h of recovery(Table 2). There were no significant differences under normoxia and hypoxia in other examined groups. Concentration of NA increased significantly (p < 0.01) immediately after the test by 55.9% in the E group under normoxia, and by 221.1% under hypoxia. There were no significant differences in other groups under all conditions (Tables 1 and 2). Under normoxia, levels of A increased significantly (p < 0.001) immediately after the test in all groups (611.5% in E athletes, 918.8% in S athletes, 491% in the C group). Moreover, in the S group, after 1 h of recovery, A concentration was significantly (p < 0.001) lower than at rest, by 837.1%, and in the E group by 68.1% compared to the level after exercise. Under hypoxia, statistical differences in the A level were only found in the E group. After exercise, the A level increased significantly (p < 0.001), by 632.9%, and decreased significantly (p < 0.001) after 1 h of recovery, by 42.8%. Additionally, in the S group, there was a tendency (p < 0.08) towards an increase in the A level immediately after the test under hypoxia, by 940%.

Discussion

Among the factors that have an influence on the combined effects of exercise and hypoxia on cognitive performance, a pivotal role is attributed to the severity or/and duration of hypoxia, as well as to the intensity or/and duration of exercise (Taylor et al., 2016). Less recognized and poorly investigated is the influence of the type of exercise and the performance level on cognitive performance in well-trained athletes. Evidence from previous studies indicates that endurance- and strength-training processes cause different adaptive changes at the CNS and peripheral levels (Leveritt et al., 1999). Taking this into account, one may argue that different types of exercise (endurance vs. strength) contribute to cognitive performance in a different way. At the CNS level, a plethora of changes in response to different types of exercise is particularly assigned to the lateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which takes part in regulating many human executive functions to prepare for situations demanding high levels of working memory, attention and cognitive flexibility (Arsten, 2011; Ludyga et al., 2016). This response can be attributed to the neural activation of dopaminergic, noradrenergic, glutaminergic systems and some neuropeptides (for example BDNF) which play a key role in activation of the prefrontal cortex and thereby cognitive function, as well as to periphery produced substances during exercise (for example lactate or ammonia) which can cross the blood-brain barrier (Arsten, 2011; Świątkiewicz et al., 2023; Zajac et al., 2015).

Our study reveals that exercise to volitional exhaustion (EVE) does not change CRT or NCR in strength-trained (S) athletes under both normoxic and hypoxic conditions. A similar phenomenon was also seen in our control (C) group. In turn, in endurance-trained (E) athletes, the EVE impaired CRT under normoxia and hypoxia (increases of 0.03 s and 0.05 s, respectively). Additionally, under hypoxia, the EVE reduced NCR by 1.7 compared to the resting state. Moreover, under hypoxia, NCR was lower in the E group after the EVE, as compared to the S (by 1.3) and C (by 1.2) groups.

On the basis of the values of maximal power reached by participants in the investigated groups, it can be assumed that exercise-induced changes in psychomotor performance occur only in response to heavily exhaustive exercise stress. This is evidenced in the present study by the rating of maximum relative (F = 3.517, p = 0.043) and absolute power that achieved the highest values in the E group as compared to the S and C groups (Table 1). In line with this assumption are the observed changes in catecholamine levels and some other biochemical and cardiovascular variables. Interestingly, the greatest increase in A and NA immediately after exercise was observed in the E group, under both hypoxia and normoxia. Moreover, under hypoxia, this increase was two and three times greater than under normoxia, respectively. At the same time, CRT was elongated immediately after maximal exercise under both normoxia and hypoxia; however, this change was significantly smaller under hypoxia compared to normoxia. In addition, the latter effect was accompanied by a reduction in NCR. Collectively, our results indicate that high-intensity exercise, performed during acute exposition to hypoxia, plays a dominant role in the pathomechanism of cognitive performance impairment. Previously, a similar effect was observed in healthy young people in response to interval exercise, as measured by the GNS task (Sun et al., 2019). Furthermore, the fact that only the E group showed cognitive impact of heavy exercise may reflect that these participants performed a higher overall motor activity (in the brain also) and exercised for a longer period of time than the other groups. The aforementioned influence on the brain could contribute to, and partially explain, differences between groups considering their cognitive function.

In our study, all groups were instructed to perform the maximal effort; therefore, we could expect similar effects at the CNS level. However, it has been shown that in endurance-trained athletes, a large percentage of exercise-induced fatigue originates from the so-called central fatigue (Tornero-Aquilera et al., 2022). In relation to our study, it can be assumed that E athletes terminated exercise more due to central fatigue than to peripheral fatigue, and this phenomenon was manifested by cognitive impairment. It is believed that strength-trained athletes are more sensitive to metabolic and chemical factors conducive to fatigue (Gandevia, 2001; Zajac et al., 2015). Hence, in our study S athletes could stop exercising without central fatigue, which resulted in no changes in cognitive function.

Consistent with the above reasoning are the catecholamines data in the E group. The higher workload in this group is associated with a significant activation of the sympathetic nervous system, both under H and N, which resulted in increased peripheral concentration of A and NA. Again, we can assume that the S and C groups stopped exercising before central fatigue occurred. Some data indicate that the effect of catecholamines on cognition depends on their level in the brain. Their release following moderate intensity exercise may promote cognitive function (Chmura et al., 1994), while an excess in A and NA can lead to the so-called neuronal noise, which is associated with inhibition of brain efficiency including cognitive impairment (McMorris et al., 2011, McMorris and Hale, 2012).

Animal studies have shown that stress induces A and DA signaling in the prefrontal cortex (Reader, 1981). Acute exposure to hypoxia has been found to be a stress factor in rats, leading to a decrease in cognitive performance, accompanied by an increase in A and DA (Miguel et al., 2018). Our study did not confirm that DA is an important player in this phenomenon, which is in line with previous research (Piotrowicz et al., 2020).

Excitatory synapses, most of all glutaminergic, dominate in the gray matter in the prefrontal cortex, indicating that excitatory neurotransmission gives rise to most of the energy requirements in this structure. Since evidence points to reduced cerebral blood flow under high intensity exercise, this can lead to reduced oxygen and glucose delivery from the vasculature into neurons and uncoupling between energy demand and supply. However, PET studies bring support to the notion that neuronal metabolic needs are met by glycolysis (Frigley and Stroman, 2011). Since the brain contains little energy reserves, the continued supply of energy substrates to neurons can be balanced by a large increase in LA production via the astrocyte-neuron lactate shuttle (Migistretti, 2009). This LA is released in the extracellular space (Bousier-Sore et al., 2006). What seems to be equally important, especially in our experimental setup, is the transport of LA from the blood via monocarboxylate transporters to the brain (Quistorff et al., 2008). In our study, the highest blood LA values in response to maximal exercise occurred in endurance-trained athletes both under hypoxic and normoxic conditions (11.6 mmol/l vs. 13.7 mmol/l, respectively), which created the most favorable conditions for the transport of this metabolite to the brain among the studied groups. A large body of evidence shows that neurons can efficiently use LA as an energy substrate (Boumezbeur et al., 2010), which is even preferentially metabolized over glucose when these substrates are simultaneously available (Itoh et al., 2003).

Interestingly, exercise-induced elevated BDNF levels did not prevent cognitive impairment in the E group under both normoxia and hypoxia conditions. Moreover, there was no effect on the improvement in CRT and NCR in the control group. Previous data indicated that one of the most important factors which can improve cognitive functions by exercise is the BDNF (Chang et al., 2012; Liu and Nusslock, 2018). However, they strongly relate to endurance training or prolonged aerobic exercises (Erickson et al., 2011; Huang and Reichard, 2009). It is believed that BDNF can improve cognition by binding to the TRKb receptor, triggering a neuroprotective cascade of intracellular signaling which involves the CaMK II, MAPK pathways (Małczyńska et al., 2019; McMorris and Hale, 2012). However, some authors suggest that the time to induce this positive effect is delayed (Slusher et al., 2018). Following this path, it would be expected that in our study, the improved CRT after rest under normoxia and hypoxia conditions was the result of the post- exercise BDNF increase. It should be noted that some authors did not report any relationship between BDNF changes and executive functions in relation to acute exercise (Piotrowicz et al., 2020; Slusher et al., 2018). Additionally, Slusher et al. (2018) showed improvement in cognitive functions after resting from short-interval exercise, but no correlation with the BDNF level, which could also confirm the delayed post-exercise effects of BDNF.

Another important finding is that the S group showed no increase in the BDNF as a result of the EVE under either normoxia or hypoxia. Under normoxia, the suppression of BDNF secretion could be explained by a significant increase in COR in this group. It is believed that stress is a factor releasing this hormone into the bloodstream (Choi et al., 2018), and its exercise level may be negatively correlated with the level of BDNF (Whiteman et al., 2014). Studies on animal models have shown that an increase in cortisol production caused a decrease in the production of BDNF in the brain (Dessypris et al., 1980; Zajac et al., 2010). It has been shown that the negative correlation can be also caused by a mutation in the BDNF encoding gene, resulting in a methionine (Met) occurrence in protein 66th residue (Val66Met) (Garcia-Suarez et al., 2020). Additionally, individuals with the aforementioned polymorphism may not show a response to exercise in the form of an increase in BDNF (Egan et al., 2003).

In the brain, ET-1 is produced by epithelial as well as neuronal and glial cells (among others), and is considered as the most potent vasoconstrictor and mitogen yet found (Silpanisong et al., 2017). ET-1 plays an essential role in the control of the brain microcirculation (Petrov et al., 2002). ET-1 stimulates secretion of nitric oxide and some other peptides participating in the hormonal control of salt and water balance (Levin, 1996). Under both physiological conditions and during exercise, ET-1 and NO play a pivotal antagonistic role in modulating the diameter of blood vessels (De Mey and Vanhutte, 2014; Rapoport and Merkus, 2018). Hypoxia and ischemia augment peripheral ET-1 secretion; however, their impact on brain ET-1 production and secretion is still poorly recognized (Levin, 1996). Importantly, during exercise, ET-1 is released to regulate vascular tone in areas that are not involved in exercise, thereby redirecting blood to organs involved in exercise, including the brain. In turn, increased exercise intensity may also lead to a balance between NO and ET-1 production due to pulsatile shear stress (De Mey and Vanhutte, 2014). The second factor enhancing production of the mentioned substances is hypoxia (Ogh et al., 2014). In our study, a significant increase in peripheral NO in the E and C groups during exercises under hypoxic condition may suggest an additive effect of these two factors in enhanced low body oxygen availability. This phenomenon is accompanied by an increase in the BDNF. The lack of significant changes in the level of ET-1 in our study indicates the lack of a meaningful role of this peptide in the regulation of cerebral microcirculation under hypoxia. This is in line with recent research indicating that acute intermittent hypoxia (IH) causes an increase in ET-1, muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) and blood pressure (BP) in healthy young men; however, the effects of IH on MSNA and BP do not occur via activation of ET receptors in untrained healthy young men (Limberg et al., 2022). The experimental evidence is more indicative of the conclusion that activated astrocytes are the most potent brain microcirculation regulators via termination ends located on significant portions of the intracerebral circulation (Nedergaard et al., 2002) and this cell population is well positioned to integrate neuronal activity and link neuronal activity to the vascular network (Ransom et al., 2003).

The correlation of BDNF production due to the NO signalization (Banoujaafar et al., 2016) is supported by the fact that in the C group, we did not observe an increase in either NO or BDNF due to the EVE. However, conversely, under normoxia, the exercise production of the aforementioned neurotrophin was increased despite there being no differences in NO concentration, which suggests that the BDNF can be synthesized independently of NO. Due to the fact that the increase in NO concentration in our study was not associated with the improvement in cognitive function in any group, it seems that this substance is not a major player in this phenomenon. It is worth mentioning that many studies which confirm the positive effect of NO on cognitive function were performed in vitro or after exogenic administration of NO (Lefferts et al., 2016). Moreover, Canossa et al. (2002) reported that only exogenously administered NO had a positive effect on the BDNF level and endogenous administration could suppress the production of this neurotrophin (Canossa et al., 2002).

Conclusions

Maximal endurance exercise to volitional exhaustion impairs cognitive function only in endurance-trained young men, which is probably caused by the coexistence of peripheral and central fatigue. Higher involvement of the sympathetic nervous system expressed by elevated blood levels of A and NA in such athletes (compared to strength-trained athletes and individuals leading a sedentary lifestyle) may also play a role in this phenomenon. Additionally, exposure to moderate hypoxia did not have a negative impact on CRT or NCR. BDNF, which is known as the main factor enhancing cognitive function, in our study did not play an important role during exercise. Moreover, both NO and ET-1 seemed to have no effect on cognition, independent of oxygen availability (normoxia vs. moderate hypoxia) and the performance level.